Intended for healthcare professionals

Education And Debate

Concept of true and perceived placebo effects

BMJ 1995; 311 doi: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.311.7004.551 (Published 26 August 1995) Cite this as: BMJ 1995;311:551
  1. E Ernst, professora,
  2. K L Resch, senior lecturera
  1. aCentre for Complementary Health Studies, Postgraduate Medical School, University of Exeter, Exeter EX24NT
  1. Correspondence to: Professor Ernst.
  • Accepted 2 June 1995

We often and wrongly equate the response seen in the placebo arm of a clinical trial with the placebo effect. In order to obtain the true placebo effect, other non-specific effects can be identified by including an untreated control group in clinical trials. A review of the literature shows that most authors confuse the perceived placebo effect with the true placebo effect. The true placebo effect is highly variable, depending on several factors that are not fully understood. A distinction between the perceived and the true placebo effects would be helpful in understanding the complex phenomena involved in a placebo response.

Interest in complex issues relating to the placebo effect has recently increased, and in 1994 most major general medical journals focused on the subject.1 2 3 4 Some of the complexities are addressed in this wry definition: placebo is the “most effective medication known to science, subjected to more clinical trials than any other medicament yet nearly always does better than anticipated. The range of susceptible conditions appears to be limitless.”5

This paper sets out to differentiate between the perceived and the true placebo effect. The former is what we commonly assume the placebo effect to be: the response observed in the placebo group of a randomised controlled trial. The true placebo effect equals this response minus other effects that often determine the outcome in all treatment groups of such studies.

Concepts

The response to the application of a placebo in a clinical trial (such as a new antihypertensive drug) is commonly equated with the placebo response--an average drop of 5 mm Hg seen in the placebo arm at the end of the treatment phase will almost automatically be attributed to the placebo effect. This is not necessarily correct. Undoubtedly a placebo effect may have contributed to the change in blood pressure, but several other factors could also have played a role.

Natural course of disease--We all appreciate that a disease or symptom may, over time, change in either direction. If, in our above example, the blood pressure would naturally decrease, such a change might exaggerate the perceived placebo effect. If it increased, the change would (partly) occlude the perceived placebo effect.

Regression towards the mean--Most biological variables fluctuate. In situations where end point variables are first measured while they are likely to be at or near a peak a second measurement would probably give a lower reading. In other words the blood pressure may, on average, be relatively high at entry and lower at the end of a treatment, quite unrelated to effects of treatment or placebo. Even though at first glance this phenomenon seems unlikely, it is very real indeed. Patients often see their doctor because their complaint is at a peak. In clinical trials patients are often included because their symptom is present to a predefined degree (such as systolic blood pressure > 160 mm Hg). In both cases a second visit would probably show that the blood pressure had fallen, thus contributing substantially to the perceived placebo effect.

Other time effects--To assume that the effect observed by administering a placebo for a given time (say four weeks) equals the placebo effect is to assume that all other factors influencing the outcome variable have remained constant. There are, however, several other potential effects that, depending on the clinical situation and setting, may change. For example, the skill of the investigator may have increased between the first and the last measurement. This may lead to detection of larger (or smaller) pathological changes than before, which can thus decrease (or increase) the perceived placebo effect. There also may be time effects related to the patients. For example, “white coat hypertension” could decrease as patients become used to having their blood pressure measured, which would spuriously increase the perceived placebo effect. In short term studies there may also be seasonal changes in blood pressure, which again could affect the size of the perceived placebo effect.

Unidentified parallel interventions--It is possible that patients, once included in a trial, become sensitised to the problem under investigation and contribute, either deliberately or unconsciously, to the clinical outcome. Hypertensive patients could, for example, reduce their salt intake, try to avoid stress, or adopt other modifications of lifestyle.

TRUE PLACEBO EFFECT

The perceived placebo effect is thus a function of several factors and cannot be equated with the true placebo effect. In trials with a parallel untreated group (no placebo orother intervention, as with “waiting list controls”) these other non-specific effects would produce the same response as in a placebo group (except, of course, for the true placebo effect). If one therefore subtracted the effects observed in such a group from those in the placebo group the true placebo effect would be identified(fig 1).

Fig 1
Fig 1

Differentiation of true placebo effect from perceived placebo effect

The size of the perceived placebo effect in relation to the true placebo effect is variable. The perceived placebo effect is equal to the true placebo effect if no effect is observed in the untreated control group, but if the perceived placebo effect is of the same size in the untreated and the placebo control group the true placebo effect is zero.

The above concept is not relevant to estimating the size of the treatment effect in placebo controlled trials. Its estimation is usually done by subtracting the perceived treatment effect from the perceived placebo effect, which results in the true treatment effect. The concept may, however, be of crucial importance for the development of treament strategies in clinical practice: the optimal benefit of a given intervention is based upon optimisation of both the specific treatment effect and the true placebo effect.4 6 It is therefore interesting that often no distinction is made between effects seen in placebo groups from those in untreated controls.

Fig 2
Fig 2

Differentiation of true treatment effect from perceived treatment effect

“Placebo” in the literature

We conducted a Medline literature search for all clinical trials and meta-analyses published during 1986-94 that included the words “placebo” and “untreated” in their summary. We read all 318 abstracts independently. Of these, 52 articles seemed (according to their abstracts) to contain both an untreated and a placebo group, and these we read in full. In fact only 12 of these did have both placebo and untreated groups. We now report on those studies that contained ratings of pain as a clinical endpoint.7 8 9 10 11 12

The table summarises key data from these publications. Generally, placebo treatment was effective in alleviating pain compared with no treatment. To elicit a true placebo effect patients must be conscious: in one trial placebo was administered directly postoperatively to sleeping patients, and no difference in pain ratings was observed between the placebo group and untreated controls.11 Objective variables may also respond to placebo treatment: this occurred in trials where swelling and C reactive protein concentration after tooth extractions were significantly different in the placebo and the untreated groups.8 9 “Physical” placebos such as detuned ultrasound equipment8 9 and sham acupuncture10 seem to be associated with more powerful true placebo effects than simple oral placebos.12

Summary of results of trials including a placebo group and an untreated control group

View this table:

Comment

It is surprising how few studies with placebo and untreated controls could be retrieved. Other search strategies were tried (such as “non-treatment” or “waiting list” in addition to “untreated”) but were not fruitful. Many authors of clinical trials simply do not make a clear distinction between the terms placebo and untreated but use these as synonyms. Clearly, this is misleading, ignores the complexity of the issue, and perpetuates widespread misconceptions about the placebo effect.

The cumulative effects of regression towards the mean, spontaneous remission, and other factors can be substantial. All may vary depending on the clinical situation and the design of the study. Our findings suggest that procedures intimately involving the patient and those that are invasive, like acupuncture or ultrasound treatment, are associated with more powerful true placebo effects than oral drug treatment. Similar findings have been shown in a recent comparative experiment: when patients were treated for primary varicose veins a topical placebo was shown to have more beneficial effects than an oral one, on both subjective and objective end points.13

A placebo response has been claimed to be a fixed constant amounting to about 30% of any given treatment effect.14 This is misleading. Neither the true nor the perceived placebo effect is such a constant. The extent of the true placebo effect depends on, among other factors, the attitude of the doctor or therapist (towards the treatment and the patient), on the attitude of the patient (towards his or her own health, the doctor or therapist, the type of treatment), on the conditioning of the patient (his or her suggestibility), and on the type of treatment (its mechanism as well as impressiveness, invasiveness, perceived plausibility, experience, cost, etc).1 15

Several other observations often made about the placebo effect seem to be debatable. True placebo effects are not inevitable and may be totally absent in certain clinical situations. They may not only affect subjective variables (such as pain) but also objectively quantifiable ones (such as C reactive protein concentration8 9). However, placebo effects on pain have repeatedly been shown to be greater than those on other symptoms.16 Placebo effects are not always beneficial and may present unwanted reactions (nocebo effects).

Conclusion

Although essential in most clinical trials, a clear distinction is still lacking between placebo and untreated control groups as well as between perceived and true placebo effects. When describing a placebo response, one must account for the natural course of the disease, the regression towards the mean, and other non-specific effects. The resulting true placebo response is highly variable between studies and may, in many cases, be substantially smaller than the perceived placebo effect. More research should be directed towards better understanding of the complex boundary conditions that may interact and determine the true placebo effect. The best way to achieve this is the inclusion of a second, untreated control group in placebo controlled trials provided that there are no ethical objections.

References