Article Text
Abstract
Rapid, large-scale uptake of new vaccines against COVID-19 will be crucial to decrease infections and end the pandemic. In a recent article in this journal, Julian Savulescu argued in favour of monetary incentives to convince more people to be vaccinated once the vaccine becomes available. To evaluate the potential of his suggestion, we conducted an experiment investigating the impact of payments and the communication of individual and prosocial benefits of high vaccination rates on vaccination intentions. Our results revealed that none of these interventions or their combinations increased willingness to be vaccinated shortly after a vaccine becomes available. Consequently, decision makers should be cautious about introducing monetary incentives and instead focus on interventions that increase confidence in vaccine safety first, as this has shown to be an especially important factor regarding the demand for the new COVID-19 vaccines.
- psychology
- public health ethics
- health promotion
Data availability statement
Materials, data, and the data analysis script are available at https://osf.io/89eux/
This article is made freely available for use in accordance with BMJ’s website terms and conditions for the duration of the covid-19 pandemic or until otherwise determined by BMJ. You may use, download and print the article for any lawful, non-commercial purpose (including text and data mining) provided that all copyright notices and trade marks are retained.
https://bmj.com/coronavirus/usageStatistics from Altmetric.com
The first vaccines against COVID-19 have been approved and it is just a matter of months before they will become globally available.1 While the rapid, large-scale uptake of vaccines is required to end the pandemic, poll data show that worldwide vaccination intentions are declining.2 In mid-October 2020, only half of German and UK respondents wanted to be vaccinated in the first 3 months after the vaccine becomes available—not enough to achieve herd immunity and sufficiently control transmission of the disease.3 4 Various interventions have been discussed to counter COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy specifically and increase vaccination intentions, including mandatory regulations5 and educational campaigns to highlight the contributions of individual vaccination to herd immunity and to fight conspiracies.6 Recently, Julian Savulescu7 advocated for monetary vaccination incentives. He argued that paying people to be vaccinated against COVID-19 is ethically just (because it is risk-neutral: being paid for an approved, marketed, otherwise unpaid-for vaccination does not increase risk) and economically reasonable (because the financial benefits of higher vaccination rates should outweigh payment costs). While monetary incentives promote certain health behaviours, such as maintaining a healthier diet8 and quitting smoking,9 virtually no empirical evidence supports the effect of payments on the uptake of new vaccines. To fill this gap and to provide decision makers with necessary evidence for designing efficient vaccination policies, we conducted a preregistered experiment (see https://aspredicted.org/yj5n2.pdf), investigating the effects of payments and communication interventions (highlighting the effects of individual vaccination on infections and herd immunity) on intentions to be vaccinated against COVID-19.
In late November 2020, N=1349 individuals from a German non-probabilistic sample, quota-representative for age×gender×education, were randomly assigned to a 2 (payment vs no payment condition) × 2 (communication vs no communication condition) factorial between-subjects design. Participants were 20–74 years old (M=47.31, SD=14.02), 666 were men and 683 women. Most participants lived alone (31%), with a partner (35%), or with a partner and children (23%). The average monthly net household income was €2835.72 (SD=€5438.50). When asked about their financial situation, only 31% reported that they never had financial worries and problems during the last 6 months. At the beginning of the experiment, participants were informed that a vaccine against COVID-19 could be approved shortly. Participants in the communication condition were also told that a rapid uptake of the vaccine was important to decrease infections, avoid new waves of the pandemic and protect those who could not be vaccinated by achieving herd immunity. Afterwards, all participants were asked to imagine they had the chance to be vaccinated against COVID-19 for free the next month. Seventy-five per cent of the participants (assigned to the payment condition) were also asked to imagine being financially rewarded for vaccination. The amount offered was randomly selected from eight alternatives (€25, €50, €75, €100, €125, €150, €175 and €200). Finally, participants were asked how likely they were to get vaccinated if they had the chance to do so the next month (on a 7-point scale from 1=not at all to 7=very much).
The results revealed an average vaccination intention of M=4.43 (SD=2.33). A regression analysis with communication, payment and their interaction predicting vaccination intention revealed no significant effects (table 1). This result did not change after replacing the binary payment factor with the actual amount offered, and when controlling for participants’ financial situation (see online supplement).
Results of regressing intention to get vaccinated against COVID-19 on communication about the need for high uptake and payments for being vaccinated
Our results challenge the suggestion that monetary incentives can foster the willingness to get vaccinated against COVID-19. Communicating the individual and prosocial benefits of high vaccination rates, payments and a combination of both strategies did not increase vaccination intentions. Perhaps the monetary incentives used in this study were too small, but larger payments undermine both economic and ethical justifications. When large sums must be paid to each person to produce a minor increase in vaccination intentions, the costs outweigh the benefits of high uptake. Furthermore, higher payments may offer disproportionate incentives to economically disadvantaged citizens, affecting their vaccination decisions but not addressing basic concerns about the vaccine.
The results should be generalised with caution. They were drawn from a German sample. While we expect similar effects for other Western countries, further studies should assess the effect of payments on vaccination intentions for other populations and cultural backgrounds. Moreover, the fictitious scenario and the assessment of vaccination intentions may not be a perfect representation of real-life vaccine decision-making. Although intention usually predicts behaviour, there may be a gap between the two, especially as social desirability of getting paid for vaccination may be low. When vaccines against COVID-19 become widely available, the behavioural consequences of monetary incentives should be investigated in field experiments.
While it may be worthwhile to further investigate the positive effects of monetary incentives for specific subgroups such as health professionals, future work should also focus on different interventions to improve vaccination intentions in the public. Other research suggests that current hesitancy about new COVID-19 vaccines is strongly related to a lack of confidence in their safety.4 10 Thus, educational campaigns addressing the safety of vaccines as we learn more about them may enable undecided people to make educated, informed decisions. Once safety concerns are reduced, monetary incentives could drive vaccine uptake. This should be addressed in future investigations.
Data availability statement
Materials, data, and the data analysis script are available at https://osf.io/89eux/
Ethics statements
Ethics approval
Our research obtained ethical clearance from the University of Erfurt’s Institutional Review Board (#20200302/20200501), and all participants provided informed consent prior to data collection.
Footnotes
Contributors PS and CB designed the research. PS, SE, LF and LK performed the research. PS planned and performed data analyses, and wrote the initial draft, which was revised and approved by all authors.
Funding The authors have not declared a specific grant for this research from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.
Competing interests None declared.
Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Other content recommended for you
- Factors influencing COVID-19 vaccination uptake among community members in Hong Kong: a cross-sectional online survey
- Vaccination practices, knowledge and attitudes regarding COVID-19 vaccines among Chinese university students: a cross-sectional study from a comprehensive university in Wuhan
- Attitudes towards vaccines and intention to vaccinate against COVID-19: a cross-sectional analysis—implications for public health communications in Australia
- Correlates of intended COVID-19 vaccine acceptance across time and countries: results from a series of cross-sectional surveys
- Nudging toward vaccination: a systematic review
- COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and vaccine passports: a cross-sectional conjoint experiment in Japan
- Healthcare workers’ (HCWs) attitudes and related factors towards COVID-19 vaccination: a rapid systematic review
- Persuasion, not coercion or incentivisation, is the best means of promoting COVID-19 vaccination
- Effect of communicating community immunity on COVID-19 vaccine-hesitant people from ethnically diverse backgrounds: an experimental vignette study in the UK
- Intention to vaccinate against COVID-19 and adherence to non-pharmaceutical interventions against COVID-19 prior to the second wave of the pandemic in Uganda: a cross-sectional study