Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Paper
Attitudes towards euthanasia in Iran: the role of altruism
  1. Naser Aghababaei
  1. Correspondence to Naser Aghababaei, Department of Clinical Psychology, Allameh Tabataba'i University, Tehran, POB 14155-8473, Iran; naseragha{at}gmail.com

Abstract

Objective Altruism is arguably the quintessential moral trait, involving willingness to benefit others and unwillingness to harm them. In this study, I explored how altruism and other personality variables relate to acceptance of euthanasia. In addition, I investigated the role of culture in attitudes to subcategorical distinctions of euthanasia.

Methods 190 Iranian students completed the Attitude Towards Euthanasia scale, the HEXACO Personality Inventory-Revised, and an interest in religion measure.

Results Higher scores on altruism, Honesty–Humility, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and religiousness were associated with viewing euthanasia as unacceptable. As expected, altruism explained unique variance in euthanasia attitude beyond gender, religiosity and broad personality factors.

Conclusions Cultural and individual differences should be taken into consideration in moral psychology research and end-of-life decision-making.

  • Moral Psychology
  • Euthanasia
  • Attitudes Toward Death
  • Psychology

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Introduction

Contemporary moral psychology emphasises the universality of moral judgement across age, gender, religion and ethnicity. People's judgement about classic dilemmas is considered to be sensitive to the same moral principles.1 Euthanasia is a moral dilemma, standing at the intersection of medicine and ethics, which has been used by moral psychologists and philosophers as a theme or stimulus to study moral judgement.2–8 Cushman and Young9 write, for example, that, ‘ordinary people's judgments are guided by explicit rules like that put forth by the American Medical Association distinguishing between “active” and “passive” euthanasia, the action/omission distinction may play a basic role in moral judgment’ (p1055). Despite philosophical arguments that deny the distinction between active and passive euthanasia,10 surveys show that the public perceive a distinction between these two whereby it may be permissible to withhold treatment and allow a patient to die (ie, passive euthanasia), but it is not permissible to take direct action to kill a patient (ie, active euthanasia).11 However, several studies in Iranian samples showed that, although ‘ordinary people’ clearly distinguish between action and omission, they do not see much of a difference between the two types of euthanasia.8 This difference may show that moral views are, at least to some extent, culture-dependent.

Attitudinal research on euthanasia, as well as moral psychology studies, has been performed extensively in the USA and Europe. With a few notable exceptions, there has been little work that directly compares euthanasia attitudes across cultures. One small qualitative study found dramatic differences among ethnic groups in the USA for starting and stopping life support, noting that the influence of culture, above religious foundations, explains key differences.12 Of particular note here, the Iranian subgroup in that study was the most supportive of starting life support and among the most disapproving of stopping it, followed by the Filipino and the Korean subgroups. There were almost no differences in the responses of Muslim and Christian Iranians, suggesting that the trend was based in culture, not a particular religion.12

Theories of moral judgement consider certain neuropsychological systems, such as affective intuition, to be central to moral judgement.13 Empirical studies have shown that individual differences in dispositional traits are predictive of ethical judgement, reasoning and activities.14 ,15 One key concept in morality is altruism, which is covered by almost every treatise on the evolution of morality.16 ,17 Findings in evolutionary psychology point to the origins of human morality in a set of emotions that make individuals care about the welfare of others (eg, kin altruism), and about cooperation, cheating and norm-following (eg, reciprocal altruism).13 The case of euthanasia provides an opportunity to explore how altruism functions in morality.

The HEXACO model partitions personality factors into six dimensions: Honesty–Humility, Emotionality, Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and Openness. Within the HEXACO model, reciprocal altruism has been represented by the Honesty–Humility and Agreeableness factors. Honesty–Humility and Agreeableness embody individual differences in two aspects of reciprocal altruism, which correspond to the tendency to cooperate with another even when (a) one could get away with defecting and (b) reciprocation has not been forthcoming. Emotionality is related to kin altruism, which involves survival of kin (especially offspring) or personal survival.18 A new facet-level scale has also been incorporated into the HEXACO model, which measures the overall altruism versus antagonism, and contains items describing sympathy, soft-heartedness and generosity. The overall tendency to be altruistic represents a blend of the Honesty–Humility, Agreeableness and Emotionality domains and shows moderate and similar correlations with these three factors, but weak correlations with the remaining HEXACO domains.18 On the basis of this distinction, I propose that Honesty–Humility, Agreeableness and altruism—defined here as a tendency involving willingness to benefit others and unwillingness to harm them—are related to viewing euthanasia as unacceptable. Emotionality is related to harm-avoidant and help-seeking behaviours that are associated with investment in kin and self.18 Because the case of euthanasia, as presented here, does not include a dimension related to administering euthanasia for one's self or kin, I expect Emotionality not to be related to euthanasia.

Thus, this study investigates the association between acceptance of euthanasia and the HEXACO model of personality, with a specific focus on altruism. I expect to find no difference between active and passive euthanasia, showing again that a deed may be considered permission (ie, letting die in passive euthanasia) and still viewed as unacceptable as an action (ie, killing in active euthanasia). Also, by adding a measure of religiosity—which is an especially salient variable for the end-of-life preferences—I will be able to flesh out the extent to which altruism is a unique predictor of attitudes toward euthanasia, if at all.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 190 student volunteers from the University of Tehran; 129 were female and 61 were male and their age range was 18–39 (mean 20.90, SD 2.96). I administered the Persian versions of these measures, which have been used previously in Iranian samples and have proven to be valid.

Participation in the study was voluntary and anonymous. This study was approved by the appropriate ethics committee of the Department of Psychology, University of Tehran.

Questionnaires

Attitude Towards Euthanasia scale

The 10-item Attitude Towards Euthanasia (ATE) scale was designed to address a number of issues central to the discussion of euthanasia, including medical condition (‘severe pain’ and ‘no recovery’), decision-making (the ‘patient's request’ or the ‘doctor's authority’), and the method of administration (‘active’ or ‘passive’ euthanasia). Items include, for example, ‘It is okay for a doctor to administer enough medicine to end a patient's life if the doctor does not believe that they will recover’ or ‘If a patient in severe pain requests it, a doctor should prescribe that patient enough medicine to end their life’. The ATE scale has been shown to have satisfactory reliability and validity.11 A five-point Likert-type scale was used, with higher scores indicating more support for euthanasia.

The HEXACO personality inventory

The HEXACO personality domains were measured using the 60-item, self-report, HEXACO Personality Inventory-Revised (HEXACO-60). This inventory has been previously vetted as a valid and reliable measure of personality. Altruism was measured with an eight-item altruism scale from the full version of the HEXACO-RI-R. This scale includes items such as, ‘I would feel very badly if I were to hurt someone’, or ‘I have sympathy for people who are less fortunate than I am’.19 ,20 A five-point Likert-type scale was used.

Interest in religion

Participants indicated their degree of interest in religion by responding to a single question: ‘How interested are you in religion?’ Answers ranged from 0 (not at all interested) to 9 (very interested). Although this single item measures religious allegiance in a relatively unobtrusive manner, it has been shown to be a sensitive index of religious motivation and displayed robust positive correlations with other religiosity measures. Positive correlations of this item with adjusted forms of religious commitment were, for example, as strong in Iran as they were in the USA.20 ,21

Results

As with previous studies in Iranian samples, the results showed no significant difference between acceptance of active (M=2, SD=1) and passive (M=2.03, SD=0.97) euthanasia (t=0.97, p=0.33). However, the difference between voluntary and non-voluntary euthanasia, represented by patient's request (M=2.21, SD=1.17) and doctor's authority (M=1.82, SD=0.93) dimensions, was highly significant (t=6.00, p=0.0001).

Table 1 provides means, SDs, bivariate and partial correlations of the ATE to the other variables. The bivariate tests show that ATE correlated negatively with altruism, religiosity, Honesty–Humility and, to a lesser extent, Conscientiousness. The Agreeableness dimension, as expected, correlated negatively with the ATE scale, but this relationship was only marginally significant (p=0.07). Of note, the gender-controlled correlations were similar to the zero-order correlations.

Table 1

Euthanasia attitude correlations with personality factors, altruism and religiousness

A hierarchical regression was used to see the unique contribution of the altruism scale by controlling for gender, religiosity and the HEXACO factor-level scales. In doing so, the altruism scale was entered (in step 2), after gender, the HEXACO factors and the measure of religiosity had been entered (in step 1). After these variables had been controlled for, altruism could explain additional unique variance in ATE (table 2).

Table 2

Results of the hierarchical regression analysis in predicting euthanasia attitude

Discussion

This study used an individual differences approach to examine the potential role of altruism in predicting judgements on euthanasia. The results show that, even after controlling for religiosity, gender and personality factors, altruism still explained additional variance in ATE. Our findings thus suggest that high levels of traits associated with altruism may underlie a pro-life stance.

Also, I found that individuals with higher scores on Conscientiousness were less supportive of euthanasia. Conscientiousness is defined by a collection of behaviours such as being organised, disciplined, dutiful and effortful. High scorers have a strong work ethic, seek order, consider their options carefully and tend to be cautious.18 In the cultural context of the sample used here, death-of-others actions tend to go against convention, religious doctrines, ethics and the law. Generally, ‘good citizens’, meaning higher scorers on Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and Honesty–Humility, tend to conform to social norms more strongly; they are less likely to endorse actions that are non-normative.15

Honesty–Humility and Agreeableness are related to religiosity.20 As we have seen in our data, these personality factors appear to capture a substantial portion of the same variance as the religiosity measure, in light of previous research showing a high level of correspondence of Honesty–Humility and Agreeableness with religiosity, combined with the fact that personality factors were not significant when religiousness was included in the regression of this study. At the same time, the statistical value of religiousness does not negate the interpretive value of the relationships between these factors and ATE. More importantly, however, the dimension of altruism explains additional variance of ATE after religiousness, gender and the personality factors have been controlled for. Thus, while religiousness adds to the explanatory power of the model, it does not totally eclipse the value of altruism as a dimension of personality related to attitude to euthanasia.

Women often score higher on altruism and altruism-related factors than men, and women tend to be less supportive of euthanasia.19 ,22 This is consistent with the negative relation between altruism and ATE in our data. However, given the nature of euthanasia, where more positive perceptions of it tend to reflect a more liberal moral framework,23 the negative relation between altruism and ATE does not confirm a previous study in Germany, which reported that individuals who score higher on altruism tend to endorse more-left-wing agendas (although euthanasia was not included in that study).24 On the other hand, a recent unpublished study by myself and colleagues on college students in the USA confirmed a negative relationship between the Euthanasia Attitude Scale (EAS) and altruism, although when the EAS was the dependent variable, altruism did not explain additional variance beyond religiosity. In total, these varying results suggest that the link between altruism and moral decision-making may be complex, with cultural contexts—where some features of the association may be similar across cultures, while others are different.

Finally, the general indifference to the distinction between passive and active euthanasia observed in the present study was expected, but does not mean that this moral distinction is not drawn in other cultures, nor does it imply the rejection of a general distinction between action and omission among the Iranian population. It simply suggests that the theoretical distinction between active and passive euthanasia may not, in practice, hold across cultures in the same way. Real world situations are influenced by personal experience, historical events and social/cultural norms, and these contexts are often omitted in artificial moral dilemmas. Because of this, moral psychologists have largely focused on artificial dilemmas as opposed to real world ones such as abortion and euthanasia.25 But this focus on artificial dilemmas means that much current research on moral psychology may shed only limited light on moral views about the difficult dilemmas faced in actual clinical contexts.

A limitation of the present study was the use of a sample consisting only of university students. Future research should examine greater diversity among individuals and study further cultural contexts. Insofar as ethics intersects cultural norms that emerge throughout the evolution of social systems and their resident populations, it is not surprising to find some differences in moral judgement between different societies with different cultural backgrounds. Additional research is needed to see if the present findings are generalisable to other cultures. In any case, the results of this study provide new evidence that culture must be taken into account in end-of-life decision-making, as well as in moral psychology research.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments, and Jason Adam Wasserman for his comments and help throughout this project.

References

Footnotes

  • Competing interests None.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

Linked Articles

  • The concise argument
    Kenneth Boyd